مرکزی صفحہ
American Journal of Psychiatry THE EFFECTS OF METHYLPHENIDATE ON SYMPTOMATOLOGy AND LEARNING IN DISTURBED CHILDREN
THE EFFECTS OF METHYLPHENIDATE ON SYMPTOMATOLOGy AND LEARNING IN DISTURBED CHILDREN
CONNERS, C. KEITH, EISENBERG, LEONآپ کو یہ کتاب کتنی پسند ہے؟
فائل کی کوالٹی کیا ہے؟
کوالٹی کا جائزہ لینے کے لیے کتاب ڈاؤن لوڈ کریں
فائل کی کوالٹی کیا ہے؟
جلد:
120
زبان:
english
رسالہ:
American Journal of Psychiatry
DOI:
10.1176/ajp.120.5.458
Date:
November, 1963
فائل:
PDF, 1.05 MB
آپ کے ٹیگز:
مسئلے کے بارے میں بتائیے
This book has a different problem? Report it to us
Check Yes if
Check Yes if
Check Yes if
Check Yes if
you were able to open the file
the file contains a book (comics are also acceptable)
the content of the book is acceptable
Title, Author and Language of the file match the book description. Ignore other fields as they are secondary!
Check No if
Check No if
Check No if
Check No if
- the file is damaged
- the file is DRM protected
- the file is not a book (e.g. executable, xls, html, xml)
- the file is an article
- the file is a book excerpt
- the file is a magazine
- the file is a test blank
- the file is a spam
you believe the content of the book is unacceptable and should be blocked
Title, Author or Language of the file do not match the book description. Ignore other fields.
Are you sure the file is of bad quality? Report about it
Change your answer
Thanks for your participation!
Together we will make our library even better
Together we will make our library even better
فائل آپ کے ای میل ایڈریس پر بھیجی جائگی۔ اسے موصول ہونے میں 5 منٹ تک کا وقت لگ سکتا ہے۔.
فائل آپ کے Kindle اکاؤنٹ پر بھیجی جائگی۔ اسے موصول ہونے میں 5 منٹ تک کا وقت لگ سکتا ہے۔.
نوٹ کریں : آپ کو ہر کتاب کی تصدیق کرنی ہوگی جسے آپ اپنے Kindle میں بھیجنا چاہیں۔ Amazon Kindle سے تصدیقی ای میل کے لیے اپنا میل باکس چیک کریں۔
نوٹ کریں : آپ کو ہر کتاب کی تصدیق کرنی ہوگی جسے آپ اپنے Kindle میں بھیجنا چاہیں۔ Amazon Kindle سے تصدیقی ای میل کے لیے اپنا میل باکس چیک کریں۔
Conversion to is in progress
Conversion to is failed
0 comments
آپ کتاب کا معائنہ کر سکتے ہیں اور اپنے تجربات شیئر کرسکتے ہیں۔ دوسرے قارئین کتابوں کے بارے میں آپ کی رائے میں ہمیشہ دلچسپی رکھیں گے۔ چاہے آپ کو کتاب پسند ہے یا نہیں ، اگر آپ اپنے دیانتدار اور تفصیلی خیالات دیںگے تو لوگوں کو نئی کتابیں ملیںگی جو ان کے لئے صحیح ہیں۔
1
|
|
2
|
|
EFFECTS THE AND OF LEARNING C. KEITH IN CONNERS, DISTURBED PH.D., Stimulant drugs have been recommended for the treatment of behavior disorders in children for several decades( 1-5). Despite this long period of use and numerous clinical reports, there have been relatively few controlled studies on these agents. Their effects have on not there is explanation In a training able to specific psychological been adequately functions explored and as yet no completely of their action. recent double-blind school for delinquents, demonstrate, on two measures, statistically satisfactory study we at a were independent significant improve- ment in behavior among children receiving dextroamphetamine(6). As a further step in the accumulation of behavioral data on the psychopharmacologic actions of stimulant drugs, we undertook the present study of methyiphenidate, an agent which has been reported to be effective in clinical trials(7, 8). METHOD Subjects: The subjects children in two residential Institution A, a Welfare for this study Prior experience were care institutions. department group foster home, serves children judged unsuitable for care in individual homes. with such to be foster children (9) led to the expectation that this group would include a large percentage of disturbed children. Institution B, a psychiatric treatment center, serves disturbed children who can be managed in an open setting. None were diagnosed as brain-damaged, defective or overfly psychotic. The children ranged in age from 7 to 15 1 Read at the 119th annual meeting of The American Psychiatric Association, St. Louis, Mo., May 6-10, 1963. The medication and placebo for the study were supplied through the courtesy of CIBA Inc. This study was supported by a grant from the Psychopharmacology Section of NIMH, MY-2583. 2Depts. of Psychiatry and Pediatrics, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, and Chil- dren’s Home, Psychiatric Service, The Johns Hopkins Hospital. 458 ON METHYLPHENIDATE Harriet Lane; AND LEON SYMPTOMATOLOGy CHILDREN1 EISENBERC, M.D.2 with a mean age of 11.9 and a standard deviation of 1.8. There were 53 boys and 28 girls, with 8 children of each sex Negro. years, General Design & Test Measures: The verbal scale of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (W.I.S.C.), with the digit span substituted for the vocabulary, was administered individually to each child. On the basis of the I.Q. distribution, the children were divided into 3 intelligence groups: low (65-79), mid (80-91), and high (94-135). Half of each intelligence group was or a placebo The tered The dose daily dose days. equal randomly sub-group. medication was on a double-blind children on drug of 10 mg. and of 30 The and designated coded basis as a drug and for adminis10 days. were begun of methylphenidate at a twice were gradually increased to a mg. twice a day for the final 5 children on placebo received an increasing number of tablets of an inert substance. There were no adverse reactions of magnitude sufficient to necessitate discontinuation of medication. All children took the medication regularly under the careful supervision of house parents. The cottage parents of group A and the child care workers 48-item symptom of group inventory(6) B filled on out a each child before treatment began and again on the last day of medication. This inventory required a judgment of symptom severity on a scale that ranged from 0 to +3. The Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale (GMAS) (10), the General Anxiety Scale for Children (GASC) (11), and a 19-item Impulsivity Scale( 12) were administered individually to each child before treatment was begun. Three learning tasks were given to each child, one before and two after treatment. Porteus Mazes( 13) and a tremorgraph test were given after treatment. A brief interview concerning subjective effects of the medication and a short questionnaire about side effects were given to 19631 C. KEITH CONNERS, AND in Table 1. TABLE Summary of Experimental SYMPTOM DIFFERENCE SCORE 1 Design and Test Procedures Low (65-79) High (94.135) WISC Symptom ratings CMAS GASC Impulsivity Scale DigitSymbol Learning * Three subjects * 13 13 13 13 13 13 Total PRE#{149}DRUG MEASURES 78 * POST-DRUG MEASURES Digit.Letter Paired Associate Learning Symptom Ratings Interview regarding side effects Tremorgraph Porteus Maze Test DigitSymbol Learning (alternate form) were held as a reserve and are not counted RESULTS Comparability of Treatment Groups on Pre-Treatment Measures: The mean age, I.Q. and initial symptom scores of the drug and placebo groups were virtually identical. No significant differences were obtained on anxiety, impulsivity or learning prior to treatment to active or inert for the groups medication. Symptom Change After Treatment: A total weighted symptom score was obtained for each child by multiplying each symptom by its severity and summing the total. The initial score was subtracted from the final to compute a difference score. Table 2 compares difference scores for the drug and placebo groups. The overall change indicates a significant improvement in the drug group (p < .026 with a one tail test since the direction of change was predicted). While the group as a whole demon- OnetaiIed strated In analyses requiring equal numbers in all groups. Three other subjects completed only part of the testing and are excluded except where their remaining scores can be validly retained. measures assigned P 10.47 Placebo group (N = 32) N Drug Placebo Drug Placebo Drug Placebo Mid (80-91) Drug group (N = 32) STAND. ERROR 2.71 BASIC DESIGN TREATMENT WISC VERBAL l.Q. 459 EISENBERC TABLE 2 Difference Scores Between InItial and Final Symptom Ratings for Children Receiving 10-Day Treatment of Methylphenidate or Placebo and raters, respectively, on the day of treatment. The basic design of the study, together with the preand post-drug measures, children last is shown LEON <.026* 5.16 hypothesis. this moderately significant difference in behavior, examination of individual protocols revealed large individual differences in responsiveness to the drug. It should be noted that one of the cottage parents in group A failed to complete the post-drug ratings for her children so that the findings are based on 64 of the original sample of 78. However, a second group of raters was available from the night shift in institution B; these ratings confirmed the findings, with a mean difference score of 10.5 (p < .06; N = 22) in favor of the drug treated children. Each item of the symptom inventory was examined in the drug separately and for differential placebo groups. change Table 3 TABLE 3 Changes in Individual Symptoms at 20% Level or Better After Drug Treatment SYMPTOMS WHICH IMPROVED CHI’ Leads into trouble Demanding Disobedient Lying Listless & apathetic Childish or immature Quarrelsome (disturbs other children) 6.13 5.74 4.27 4.15 2.00 1.77 1.70 <.02 <.02 <.05 <.05 <.20 <.20 <.20 P 5.86 3.00 1.70 <.02 <.10 <.20 SYMPTOMS WHICH WORSENED Nailbiting Stomach aches Anxious, fearful lists 20% the items level of which discriminated at the probability or better. The largest changes appear to occur on outwardly-directed symptoms which would be disturbing to caretakers. It is of interest that three symptoms and fearful-anxious) (nailbiting, were stomach aches reported as worse 460 EFFECTS OF METHYLPHENIDATE ON in the drug group at the 20% level or better. Although the extent of these changes is not sufficient to place high reliability in them, they suggest the possibility of an increase in anxiety from the medication. Learning and Maze Performance: institution the Porteus better subjects Maze scores performance (Table 4), A. tributable These findings in age, to differences are I.Q., not atrace or sex. They are consistent with clinical observations made by those who tested the children. An additional finding is the higher “lie” score on both anxiety scales for the group B cases (p < .10) which may indicate that they were more defensive in re- On both the paired-associate learning tests and on the Porteus Mazes, the drug group demonstrated significantly better scores. On the learning tasks, however, analyses of covariance indicated that errors were significantly reduced (p < .05) only in the last block of learning trials. This effect was more marked among the B cottage children, i.e., those who were presumably sicker. Moreover, significantly drug-treated F November SYMPTOMATOLOGY porting anxiety symptoms. Thus, the anxiety scales may not be an adequate measure of the actual level of anxiety in these more disturbed children. Side Effects and Subjective Response: The children were questioned individually regarding problems of sleep, fatigue, anor- exia, nervousness, dizziness, ataxia, bowel problems and bladder problems. Only appetite was significantly affected by the showed by the an effect TABLE 4 Porteus Maze Test QuotientScores for Drug and Placebo Groups DRUG Low lQ (N = 28) 11.88 Mid lQ (N = 24) Hi lQ (N = 27) Total (N = 79) 11.08 12.42 11.79 PLACEBO ST. ERROR 9.04 12.61 1.07 1.20 .798 10.42 .636 9.60 which drug, I.Q. also suggested an interaction with That is, the children of low and average I.Q. displayed greater gains from drug therapy than did those of high I.Q. These results are reported and analyzed in greater detail in a separate report( 14). Anxiety and ImpuL9ivity Scaks: In view of the large individual differences in symptom improvement scores noted earlier, we examined the anxiety and impulsivity scales to determine whether variance on these initial measures might account for dif- reporting ferential responsiveness. However, we were unable to find any correlation between initial status on these instruments and any measures of improvement. Their failure to relate to initial symptom scores indicates that they are measuring attributes of the children different from those registered by the symptom inventory. It is, however, of some interest more disturbed children (those tion B) scored higher on the scale (p < .001) and lower on scales (p < .005) than the foster that the in instituimpulsivity the anxiety children in with T 2.13 1.70 <.05 <.10 n.s. 2.15 <.05 <1 30 of the drug subjects (79%) some loss of appetite as opposed to only 3 placebo the drug-treated difficulty going to placebo group had ilar results were parents’ report of questions regarding calmer changes stitution P subjects (7.3%). Seven of children reported some sleep, while none in the any such problem. Simobtained from cottage side effects. A series of general improvement, behavior, adverse effects and mood were asked of the raters in inA. When a +1 was assigned for a positive effect and a -1 for a negative effect, the total score for the drug group was significantly better than for the placebo group (p < .05). Similar questions were asked of the children from both institutions, with the drug group reporting “feeling happier” since taking medication (p < .036), but not differing from the placebo group on the total score. DISCUSSION The ventory, difference as well scores as the on the more symptom global in- judg- ‘:r T: - C. KEITH CONNERS, AND ments asked for statistically of the raters, provide evidence significant improvement following treatment with methylphenidate for the children in both institutions. Such rating methods are, however, subject to rater bias. The high incidence of side effects (70%) in the drug group leads to the possibility that, although the medication was coded and administered “blind,” the raters might have been able to recognize those on active drug on the basis of the side effects. this as the are two sets Against findings same scales explanation of data. discriminated medication subjects in an earlier amphetamine when for First, study subjective the the on active with dextro- complaints were fewer( 6). Moreover, an independent measure in that study, peer ratings by the children, was in agreement with the symptom scores. Second, we have independent evidence in this study of some objective improvement in the drug treated subjects on two learning tasks and on the Porteus Maze. This increases our confidence in ascribing the difference registered on the symptom inventory to the effects of the drug. The findings in the present study are thus in accord with clinical impressions reported in the literature(7, 8). They are also in agreement with those from our concurrent study of methylphenidate, phenobarbital and placebo in the outpatient treatment of hyperkinetic and defective children, a study in which improvement worsening the in stimulant produced and the symptoms( sedative reliable reliable 15). The learning test and Porteus Maze results are of particular interest in indicating improved performance on the drug, with a suggestion on the Porteus Maze of a drug-intelligence interaction. The multiplicity of factors that can influence learning precludes any firm explanation of these findings, but they do offer an avenue for a more detailed exploration of the psychopharmacologic action of methylphenidate. It is tempting to relate the improvement in Porteus Maze performance to a decrease in impulsivity, since impulsivity in a subject will interfere with his performance. We ‘had expected that methylphenidate might high provide maximal on impulsivity benefit and to the low on anxiety child and LEON 481 EISENBERG a contrary effect on the youngthe opposite rating. The failure of the anxiety and impulsivity scales employed in this study to correlate with maze scores might have ster with or learning scores might reflect either the inadequacy of the scales or the incorrectness of ‘the hypothesis. There is at least the suggestion that the anxiety and impulsivity scales are not measuring what they purport to measure from their lack of correlation with the symptom inventory on which we were able to detect improvement in behavior. The mines available literature and methyiphenidate on the amphetareveals little directly applicable to children, save for the previously mentioned therapeutic trials( 1-5, 7,8) in which the emphasis has been on the paradoxical duced decrease by these in agents hyperkinesis classed (15). In the one clinical elucidation of physiological action, Laufer and his investigated mine the study directed at mechanisms of coworkers( 16, 17) of dextroamphetathe threshold to photoof the EEC in children impulse disorders. The thresholds than a com- effect in modifying metrazol activation with hyperldnetic patients had lower parison children. pro- as stimulants group of non-hyperkinetic disturbed Following the administration of “clinically mine, the significant olds and efficaceous doses” of amphetahyperkinetic children displayed a increase in photometrazol thresh- achieved the range characteristic for the comparison group. The authors suggest that the drug, “perhaps by raising the level of synaptic resistance,” alters the functions of the diencephalon and reticular activating system, a tenable but unproved hypothesis. Weiss and Laties( 18) have provided complete review of the effects of amphetamines on performance in adult subjects. They conclude that decrease reaction and coordination, havior, counteract mood. Although problem either performance solving normal can: time, improve steadiness enhance monitoring befatigue, and elevate amphetamines appear to hasten the acquisition ing, there is no evidence intellectual amphetamines a or subjects of simple that they conditionimprove (as measured intelligence tests) or mental defectives, by in 462 EFFECTS OF METHYLPHENIDATE despite early claims based upon uncontrolled studies. However, it should be noted that there have been few attempts to meas- ure new the drugs learning as opposed on already problem solving acquired to effects knowledge for of or capacity. results must be viewed cautiously in of the influence of species, age, sex, environmental context and method of measurement on activity levels( 19). Prefrontal lesions in monkeys appear to release hyperkinetic behavior which is appreciably reduced by either amphetamine or methylphenidate(20). At the same time, cortical lesions have been shown to lower convulsive thresholds to amphetamines(21). In the rat, cortical lesions did not alter activity level per se but instead sensitized the animal to amphetamine-induced hyperidnesis (22). It should be noted that in the animal species studied, just as in the human, there is a wide range of individual variation(23). In general, it is the animals with the most active pre-drug condition who display the greatest response to drugs( 19). in impulsivity (as measured by the Porteus Maze) and in aggressive behavior (as measured by symptom ratings) registered in our patients is compatible with the hypothesis that hances the action of systems (24). The methylphenidate eninhibitory controlling same explain the improvement stemming from suppression ity. However, a second creased alerting-is we are to judge performance on mechanism might in learning as of distractibil- mechanism-inprobably active here if from reports of human stimulants( 18) and from the remarkably alert attitudes of primates whose spontaneous pacing has been decreased by these agents as opposed to tranquilizers(20). However, the available evidence is both too limited and too contradictory to permit any firm conclusions on the mode by which, and the loci at which, sympathomimetic amines action. their further study an adequate it F November of the theory is perhaps cautionary The view decrease SYMPTOMATOLOGY ical functions influenced formation which will Animal studies on the action of stimulants have centered mostly on their effects on motor behavior, either in the intact animal or in the experimentally brain-damaged one. The ON exert their psychopharmacologic The present findings, in addition to clinical significance, offer an avenue point specffic by these be psycholog- drugs, necessary can be constructed. worthwhile to make that while in- before the some evidence in the present study attests to statistically significant changes in our patients, the wide individual differences in responsiveness and the consequent smallness of the overall changes indicates that the practical or clinical significance of the drug methylphenidate must still be determined for care- fully wide selected clinical populations over a range of conditions and drug dosage. Future studies of the personality and other factors influencing drug responsiveness need to be done before blanket endorsements can be given for general clinical use. SuMuw This study investigated the effects of methylphenidate administered double-blind over a 10-day period to two groups of emotionally disturbed children (total N = 81). Symptom ratings by house parents and child care workers were obtained before treat- ment and on the last day of treatment. Total symptom scores were improved in the drug group Changes mostly at a in with statistically individual outwardly significant level. symptoms occurred directed symptoms such hints only as “leads into trouble,” and there were that anxiety showed an increase. The adverse side effect was a high report (70%) of appetite loss in the drug group. Objective performance ment in the measures of learning also shOwed reliable drug group, minimizing sibility that rater biases. itial level and maze improve- the posdue to of inimpulsivity were improvement in symptom changes were Questionnaire measures of anxiety and not related to symptomatic the drug group, and some doubt as validity of these instruments for the children studied was raised by the The results agree with previous studies activation The wide and are interpreted to the type of results. clinical as reflecting an of inhibitory controlling systems. individual variation in response to the drug indicates further research on individual difference factors is required, as well as caution in the application of the drug in clinical situations. 19631 C. KEITH CONNERS, BIBLIOGRAPHY 1. Bradley, C.: Am. J. Psychiat., 94: 577, 1937. 2. Bradley, C., and Bowen, M.: Am. J. Orthopsychiat., 11: 92, 1941. 3. Bender, L., and Cottington, F.: Am. J. Psychiat., 99: 116, 1942. 4. Bradley, C. : Pediatrics, 5: 24, 1950. 5. Pasamanick, B.: Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., 65: 752, 1951. 6. Eisenberg, L., et at.: Am. J. Orthopsychiat., 33: 431, 1963. 7. Knobel, Mauricio, and Lytton, George J.: Dis. Nerv. Syst., 20: Aug. 1958. 8. Knobel, Mauricio, et at.: A.M.A. Arch. Gen. Psychiat., 1: 310, 1959. 9. Eisenberg, L.: Am. J. Orthopsychiat., 32: 5, 1962. 10. Castaneda, A., et at.: Child Devel., 27: 317, 1956. 11. Sarason, S. B., et at.: Anxiety in Elementary School Children: A Report of Research. New York: J. Wiley, 1960. 12. Sutton-Smith, B., and Rosenberg, B. C.: J. Genet. Psychol., 95: 211, 1959. 13. Porteus, S. D.: The Maze Test and Clinical Psychology. Palo Alto: Pacific Books, 1959. 14. Conners, C. K., et a!.: The Effects of Methylphenidate on Paired Associate Learning and Porteus Maze Performance in Disturbed Children. Submitted. 15. Whitehouse, D., et at.: A Double Blind Study of Phenobarbital, Methyiphenidate and Placebo in Hyperkinetic Children. To be published. 16. Laufer, M. W., et at.: Electroenceph. Clin. Neurophysiol., 6: 1, 1954. 17. Laufer, M. W., et a!.: Psychosom. Med., 19: 38, 1957. 18. Weiss, B., and Laties, V. G.: Pharmacol. Rev., 14: 1, 1962. 19. Irwin, S.: Rev. Canad. Biol., 20: 239, 1961. 20. Davis, G. D.: Am. J. Physiol., 188: 619, 1957. 21. Blum, R. A., et at.: Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., 64: 685, 1950. 22. Adler, M. W.: J. Pharmacol. Exp. Therap., 134: 214, 1961. 23. Cole, J., and Glees, P.: J. Ment. Sci., 103: 406, 1957. 24. Eisenberg, L.: Behavioral Manifestations of Cerebral Damages in Childhood. To be published. DISCUSSION JOEL This P. study ZRULL, has M.D. what can (Ann Arbor, be called Mich.) the new AND LEON EISENBERG 463 look in methods of assessing the effects of drugs on human behavior (and in this case specifically the behavior of children). In the past, most have been content with assessing the effects of a drug by a clinical evaluation of the person’s behavior while on the drug (by the doctor alone). It is becoming increasingly apparent that this is not entirely reliable nor does it give an adequate estimate of the drug’s potentiality. The method employed here allows a wider view of the drug’s effects as well as an inherent cross validation of the individual tests. No doubt one of the most important distinctions this type of study points up is that when trying to alter behavior with a drug it is the behavior in the every day setting that is to be affected and not the behaviOr in the doctor’s office. There is now a tendency to use other than strictly medical techniques to determine qualitatively and quantitatively the effects in the clinical trials of drugs. This method 11lustrates the use of personnel other than physicians to rate these children (possibly akin to using parents or school teachers on an outpatient basis) and using psychological testing to its greatest advantage (i.e., selectively to suit what the hypothetical results might be). The effectiveness of studies with drugs will be extended when more thorough evaluations on outpatients are undertaken. The use of personnel such as parents, school teachers, psychologists as well as the physician will allow a sampling of the child’s behavior in many areas. Besides this the attitudinal changes in the parents and teachers produced by the child taking a drug will be available for evaluation. This has proved very interesting in our studies, and seems to bear a great deal on drug effectiveness. One of the most appealing advantages in using central nervous system stimulants in behavior disorder children and in particular those with learning difficulties, since a great share of them have these difficulties, is the fact that one is not depressing the children with a sedative type drug. This particular action itself is worthy of a good deal of study. The whole area of concern over what drug effects there are on learning processes and developmental processes can be hopefully alleviated or consolidated by studies such as this. The fact that these children tended to show more fearfulness as their hyperactivity and impulsivity decreased seems understandable if one takes these latter types of symptoms as releases for anxiety that were then removed. This brings one to wonder generally what the 464 EFFECTS OF METHYLPHENIDATE hyperkinetic syndrome is. Considering that it is seen cross-cutting the range of classification it could be hypothesized to have as its origin something common to all these disorders. No doubt anxiety underlies it to a great degree. It has been our finding that no matter what produces the anxiety, that is, neurotic conifict, showering impulses, or difficulty in aligning perceptions, the resultant hyperkinetic syndrome can respond to a CNS stimulant. In spite of this, many of the children we studied proved amenable to the drug at one time and then not at another. Besides this, two children with the same diagnosis might not both be helped by the CNS stimulant. This speaks for the necessity of building new symptom rating scales or methods of assessing symptoms, through studies such as these. Some factor other than the type of anxiety seemingly controls whether there will be a response or not. The question of alerting versus inhibitory activity of the CNS stimulants is intriguing and not adequately studied. The possibility of both mechanisms operating is not out of the question. The alerting theory is appealing because of what one knows of the action in adults-it is obvious that they are made more active and stimulated by the drugs. Along with this, of course, the ability to be aware of and to blot out unnecessary stimuli, which ON SYMPTOMATOLOGY [November to some extent implies the inhibitory activity, can cut down on the need to react impulsively to each stimulus and thus results in less hyperactivity as well as increased efficiency in psychic functioning. A greater awareness of one’s surroundings allows the individual to view them as less hostile and thus the need for aggressive behavior is decreased. This same alerting may be involved in the apparent increase in learning capacities noted in the paper. The authors are correct, however, in their statement that the evidence is too limited to permit firm conclusions. Many might argue that 10 days is hardly enough to determine whether the effects seen are strictly drug effect. However, with methylphenidate as well as the amphetamines a very short period is all that is necessary to build up blood levels sufficient to cause physiologic changes. The study itself has adequately allowed for the placebo effect of taking a pill. It has been our own finding, however, that on lower dosages on an outpatient basis all drugs that are given first are reacted to the same as a placebo. Only later can one discern the true drug effect. There is a further consideration which might eliminate matching problems in the groups, that of using the subjects as their own controls by giving the drugs and placebo in a cross-over pattern.